Should Indonesian Kids Under the Age
of 10 Learn to Communicate in English?
Eka Sugeng Ariadi
Kemenag Class 2015 – NIM. 157835408
The State University of
Surabaya
Due on the title above, readers may
pose various interpretations in their mind. Drawing proper answers (moreover to
dispute it) in which age period is better for Indonesian kids to start learning
to communicate in English, under or upper the age of ten, is always
interesting, challenging and emerging countless debate. Fromkin, Rodman, &
Hyams (2011)
notice that the factors of age, talent, motivation, etc, probably take into
account the succeed of second language (L2) learners to learn/acquire a new
language. Therefore, among Indonesian scholars, it is
indispensable to expose deeper and clearer about this case by comparing and
contrasting equally the both sides. Obviously, each side would be enclosed with
reasons and argumentations. Impersonal, at the end of this essay, I -as an
indigenous person- tend to say that Indonesian kids under 10 should not learn
to communicate in English.
Kids Under 10 Years Should Learn
to Communicate in English
Several linguists and
educators powerfully believe that kids in this period have a lot of advantages
to their language acquisition and its flourishment if they attempt to start
learning (likewise communicating) other languages as L2 or foreign language (FL).
Edelenbos, et al. (2006, p.148) convince us that
“young children can benefit in a number of ways if their initial experience is
not restricted to listening, speaking and
doing but
includes a gradual and systematic introduction to reading and writing from an
early point”. From ‘an early
point’ of young children herein can be understood since the birth until the age
of 12. As Yule, Fromkin,
Rodman, & Hyams (2011, p.22) claim
that “during
the
critical period, language acquisition proceeds easily, swiftly, and without external intervention, meanwhile after this period, the
acquisition of grammar is difficult and, for most individuals, never fully
achieved”. According to them, kids under the age of five mostly are able to
comprehend language grammar, “i.e. conjoining sentences, asking questions,
using appropriate pronouns, negating sentences, forming relative clauses, and
inflecting verbs and nouns and in general have the creative capacity to produce
and understand a limitless number of sentences.” (p.324).
These beneficial is merely endorsed by kids’ affective
filter, as Richards & Rodgers (2002) say that “affective filter (e.g., fear or embarrassment) may
account for children's apparent superiority to older acquirers of a second
language”. (p.183). Brown (2006) also
informs us that “There are dozens of salient differences between first and
second language learning: the most obvious difference, in the case second language learning, is
the cognitive and affective
contrast between adults and children” (p.34). Thus, in contrary to adult
learner, affective filter does not take a significant account to acquire and learn
new languages.
In general, few educators have done some observations and announced that children are better
language learners than adults (Brown, 2006., Horn & Ward, 2006). The insight of American
Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASLHA) also observes that children are prominently
able to learn more than two languages (Richard, 2010). Not only to learn but
also to communicate with other people as well. Again, Yule
(2010) assures that “by the age six, children already know a lot about
how to use language and they have begun to master many of the details of how to
participate in and manage conversations.” (p.165).
In accordance with those arguments, people then believe
that children (wherever they live, particularly in Indonesia) should learn to communicate with their L2/FL
as earlier as possible. Hence, in Indonesian context, prior to the implementation of 2013 curriculum and the
elimination of English subject at
elementary school, he had ‘forced’ his citizen overall to learn English since at very
young age. Consequently, this policy then affected Indonesian
family to send their kids to learn and to communicate in English attentively in
many courses, even send them to study abroad.
Kids After 10 Years Should
Learn to Communicate in English
It has been well understood
that
learning to communicate in English as L2/FL, is better occupied
during “the critical period” (Yule, Fromkin, Rodman, & Hyams,
2011) or “the sensitive period” (Yule, 2010). Whereas, on contrary side, Richard’s (2010) finding shows that in bilingualism older learner may also much
perceive much beneficial than younger learner. Moreover, if we little bit discuss
further about the function of learning language. In the insight of Pragmatics,
the speaker/writer should maintain good communication with the listener/reader,
in vice versa in form of verbal and non-verbal communication. Carter
(1997) maintains that, “it is even more essential that students should
understand the contractedness of texts from both the inside, as writers
themselves, and from the outside, as textual critics” (p.16). In other words, language learners
are recommended and suggested
learning to communicate with the target language (TL) entirely.
As a matter a fact, good communication in L2 as TL probably
could only be practiced smoothly by older learners. Slavin
(2003. p.44) reassures that “notion of a Zone of Proximal Development
(ZPD-Vygotsky) is an important facet of social
constructivism because it describes tasks that a child cannot yet do alone but could
do with the assistance of more competent peers or adults.” Therefore, Brown (2006) says “Adults have developed even
greater concentration and so have greater ability for rote learning eventhough rote learning is only for short-term memory or
for somewhat artificial
purposes” (p.67). In addition, Kumaravadivelu (2006) concludes that
adult learners perceive high level proficiency in learning L2 than younger
learners.
Finally, based on the arguments
above, simply it persuades us that learning to communicate in English after the
critical/sensitive period is much recommended for L2/FL learners, particularly for Indonesian learners. Yule (1996) convinces that starting
learning L2 in adult ages is better than in kids ages.
The best period of learning to
communicate in English for Indonesian Kids
Since “learning language means learning to communicate,” (Richards
& Rodgers, 2001, p.156), and “communicating
means analysing of what people mean by their utterances than what the words or
phrases in those utterances might mean by themselves,” (Yule,
1996, p.3), adolescence
is the best
period of learning English (as an TL/L2/FL for Indonesian
learners in order to be able to communicate with. Learning language fruitfully
at the critical period may become substantial clues as the best age for kids to
learn their L2, nevertheless those clues are insufficient to support the
position. The weakness of this position is argued by Linse (2005) in her
research finding that “A child may learn to read at a very early age and
have developed advanced cognitive skills but behave in ways that are viewed as
emotionally and socially immature,” (p.5). Further, Brown (2007, p.87) has warned us that
“There are actually many instances of 6- to 12-year-old children
manifesting significant difficulty in acquiring a second language for multitude
of reasons”. Therefore, “Children
under age 10, an
early start by itself however guarantees nothing; it needs to be accompanied
minimally by good teaching, by a supportive environment and by continuity from
one year to the next, taking children smoothly from pre-primary to primary, and
from primary into secondary education,” (Edelenbos, et al., 2006).
In this present essay, it necessary to note what
Kumaravadivelu (2006)
have done. She clearly
states “Those
in favour of the “younger is better” case argued that L2 development by
children and adults might actually involve different processes; the former
utilizing innate properties of language acquisition as in L1 acquisition, the
latter employing general problem-solving abilities, and thus accounting for the
differential effect of age. However,
there are others who suggest that “older is better” because older learners have
cognitive and literacy skills that tend to enhance their L2 development” (p.32). Tembe
& Norton’s
(2011) study have shown
the importance of the role of community and with
some exceptions, adults need more consciousness attention to learn
second/additional language.
Lastly, based on the given arguments above and as already stated in
prompt to, the writer proposes to stand in line with the latter position. It means the worthy of learning to
communicate in English as L2/FL for Indonesian kids after the critical period/adolescence/adult is more
reliable. Hereafter, the best action to take for Indonesian L2
learners is asking them
seriously to learn to communicate in English when they are adult or starting to study at Junior
High School. This
decision will be the same as applied at the United
States that children do not start learning additional languages until middle
school or college. While at
China, the policy of compulsory of learning English since early ages has given
rise to new inequities and is far from being a means to fair development. And in Indonesia, itself, new primary
school curriculum has been dropped English as one of elementary subject (Pasassung, 2003).
REFERENCES
Brown,
H. Douglas. (2006). Principles of language learning and Teaching, Fifth
Edition.
USA; Pearson Education, Inc.
Brown,
H. Douglas. (2007). Teaching by Principles, An Interactive Approach to
Language
Pedagogy, Third Edition. USA; Pearson Education, Inc
Carter,
Ronald. (1997). Investigating English Discourse; Language, literacy and
literature.
London & NY; Routledge.
Edelenbos, Peter., Johnstone, Richard., & Kubanek, Angelika. (2006). The main
pedagogical
principles underlying the teaching of languages to very young learners;
Languages for the children of Europe; Published Research, Good
Practice
& Main Principles. EUROPEAN COMMISSION;
Education and Culture Culture and Communication Multilingualism Policy
Fromkin,
Victoria., Rodman, Robert., & Hyams, Nina. (2011). An Introduction to
Language,
Ninth Edition. Canada; Wadsworth, Cengage Learning
Horn,
Laurence R. & Ward, Gregory (ed.). (2006). The Handbook of Pragmatics.
USA;
Blackwell Publishing L.td
Kumaravadivelu, B. (2006). Understanding Language Teaching; From Method to
Postmethod. New Jersey; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers
Linse,
Caroline, T. (2005). Practical English Language Teaching: Young Learners.
NY;
McGraw-Hill ESL/ELT
Pasassung,
Nikolaus (2003). Teaching English in an
“Acquisition-Poor Environment”:
An Ethnographic Example of
a Remote Indonesian EFL Classroom.
Unpublished PhD Dissertation, University of Sydney.
Richard, Johnstone. (2010). Introduction; In Learning
through English; Policies,
Challenges and Prospects Insights
from East Asia. UK; British Council.
Richards,
Jack C. & Rodgers, Theodire S. (2002). Approaches and Methods in
Language
Teaching, second edition. USA; The Cambridge University
Press.
Slavin, R. (2003). Educational psychology: Theory and practice. Boston: Allyn and
Bacon
Tembe,
Juliet & Norton, Bonny. (2011). English education, Local
Languages and
Community Perspectives in Uganda. In
Hywel Coleman(ed.) Dreams and
Realities:
Developing Countries and the English Language. UK;
British
Council.
Yule, George. (1996). Pragmatics. New York; Oxford University
Press.
Yule,
George. (2010). The Study of Language Fourth edition. USA; Cambridge
University Press
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar